Social anxiety and disordered eating

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Eating Behaviors
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/eatbeh
Social anxiety and disordered eating: The influence of stress reactivity and
self-esteem
Jessica Lyn Ciarma⁎,1, Jaya Miriam Mathew
Australian Catholic University, 115 Victoria Parade, Fitzroy, Vic 3065, Australia
A R T I C L E I N F O
Keywords:
Social anxiety
Eating disorder
Self-esteem
Stress
A B S T R A C T
While previous research indicates a strong link between social anxiety and disordered eating, more research is
needed in order to understand the mechanisms that underlie this relationship. Given that stress is often
implicated in disordered eating, it was hypothesised that ones reaction to stress (i.e. stress reactivity) would
mediate the relationship between social anxiety and disordered eating. Similarly, given that low self-esteem is
commonly reported in both those with social anxiety and eating disorders, it was hypothesised that self-esteem
would also mediate the relationship between social anxiety and disordered eating. In order to test this, an online
survey measuring social anxiety, disordered eating, stress reactivity and self-esteem, was administered to 282
participants in the community, aged between 18 and 35 years. Results showed that self-esteem and a reactivity
to stress during social conflict – but not during negative social evaluations – partially mediated the relationship
between social anxiety and disordered eating. These findings demonstrate that low self-esteem and interpersonal
conflict are powerful mechanisms that can maintain eating disorder psychopathology in those who are socially
anxious. This highlights the importance of ensuring that these mechanisms are sufficiently addressed in eating
disorder prevention and treatment programs.
1. Introduction
The relationship between social anxiety and disordered eating is
well-documented. Several studies have reported positive correlations
between symptoms of social anxiety and disordered eating in clinical
and community samples (Grabhorn, Stenner, Stangier, & Kaufhold,
2006; Hinrichsen, Wright, Waller, & Meyer, 2003;
Levinson & Rodebaugh, 2012; McLean, Miller, & Hope, 2007; Menatti,
DeBoer, Weeks, & Heimberg, 2015; Wonderlich-Tierney & Vander Wal,
2010). Studies on clinical samples have reported high rates of comorbidity
between social anxiety disorder (SAD) and eating disorders. For
instance, Kaye, Bulik, Thornton, Barbarich, and Masters (2004),
reported that out of 672 individuals with an eating disorder, approximately
20% met criteria for SAD. This appears to be much higher than
in community samples, where the lifetime and 12-month prevalence of
SAD were estimated to be approximately 12.1% and 7.1%, respectively
(Ruscio et al., 2008). Further, SAD is the most common anxiety disorder
experienced in individuals with eating disorders, and studies have
reported the lifetime prevalence of SAD to be around 39% and 17%
among individuals with Anorexia Nervosa (Halmi et al., 1991) and
Bulimia Nervosa (Brewerton et al., 1995), respectively. According to
retrospective research, SAD is purported to precede the onset of eating
disorders, prompting some researchers to argue that SAD is an
important risk factor for eating disorder onset (Bulik, Sullivan,
Fear, & Joyce, 1997). The significance of SAD in individuals with eating
disorders is further underscored by the fact that symptoms of social
anxiety has been shown to predict client disengagement during eating
disorder treatment (Goodwin & Fitzgibbon, 2002).
Some theoretical perspectives, such as Interpersonal theory and
Emotion Regulation theory, offer explanation to the link between social
anxiety and disordered eating. According to Interpersonal theory,
relationship difficulties may result in emotional distress, and therefore,
binge eating may be used as a coping mechanism to alleviate such
distress (Ansell, Grilo, & White, 2012). Similarly, Emotion Regulation
theory suggests that disordered eating represents a maladaptive behavioural
response, employed to reduce heightened emotions (Hilt,
Hanson, & Pollak, 2011; Polivy & Herman, 1993). While restrictive
eating may reduce negative affect by allowing a sense of control to
be gained (Hatch et al., 2010; Lask, 2000), binge eating may be used as
a means to temporarily numb negative emotions (Hilt et al., 2011).
Therefore, consistent with the Transdiagnostic theory of eating disorders,
it is believed that regardless of the specific symptoms present,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.03.011
Received 17 October 2016; Received in revised form 15 March 2017; Accepted 21 March 2017
⁎ Corresponding author.
1 Permanent address: 1 Derby St., Kew, Vic 3101, Australia.
E-mail addresses: jessica[email protected] (J.L. Ciarma), [email protected] (J.M. Mathew).
Eating Behaviors 26 (2017) 177–181
Available online 08 April 2017
1471-0153/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
MARK
all eating disorders have similar underlying psychopathology (Fairburn,
Cooper, & Shafran, 2003).
However, despite both theory and research indicating a clear link
between social anxiety and disordered eating, limited research has
tested mediation models to understand the mechanisms that may be
responsible for this relationship. Research has pointed toward the role
of fear of negative evaluations (Levinson & Rodebaugh, 2012; Menatti
et al., 2015), emotional suppression (McLean et al., 2007), shame
(Grabhorn et al., 2006), and emotional coping strategies (Hinrichsen
et al., 2003; Wonderlich-Tierney & Vander Wal, 2010) as factors that
might explain this relationship. However, Menatti et al. (2015) noted
that further research exploring mediating mechanisms is required to
fully understand the cognitive and behavioural processes implicated in
social anxiety and disordered eating.
The extent to which one interprets external situations as harmful or
unmanageable, known as stress reactivity (Scholtz, Yim, Zoccola,
Jansen, & Schulz, 2011), may potentially mediate the relationship
between social anxiety and disordered eating. Disordered eating often
emerges during, or is exacerbated by, stressful situations (e.g., transition
to university, interpersonal conflict, puberty onset), particularly in
individuals who have difficulty dealing with negative affect and stress
(Barker & Galambos, 2007; Delinsky & Wilson, 2008; Klump, Perkins,
Burt, McGue, & Iacono, 2007; Lieberman, Gauvin, Bukowski, & White,
2001). Moreover, experimental research has shown that inducing
interpersonal stress via provoking feelings of loneliness and rejection,
which are characteristic of those with social anxiety, led to an increase
in disordered eating behaviours and cognitions in individuals with
bulimia nervosa (Tuschen-Caffier & Vögele, 1999). Considering that
disordered eating may function to regulate negative emotions in
individuals with elevated levels of social anxiety, it is therefore
plausible that this relationship depends, at least in part, on individual
differences to stress reactivity (Hinrichsen et al., 2003). According to
Scholtz et al. (2011), stress reactivity in social situations can either be
in result of social evaluations (i.e. losing self-confidence in response to
negative social evaluations) and social conflict (i.e. feeling upset and
annoyed in response to social conflict or criticism). However, no studies
to our knowledge have investigated how both forms of stress reactivity
might mediate the relationship between social anxiety and disordered
eating.
Self-esteem might also be another variable underlying the relationship
between social anxiety and disordered eating. Self-esteem refers to
a person’s level of self-acceptance, which stems from an appraisal of
global self-worth, attractiveness, competence, and ability to achieve
one’s own aspirations (Robson, 1988). Self-esteem is significantly lower
in individuals with eating disorders relative to controls (Fairburn et al.,
2003), and several studies have reported negative relationships between
self-esteem and disordered eating, in both clinical and community
samples (Lampard, Byrne, McLean, & Fursland, 2011; Lampard,
Tasca, Balfour, & Bissada, 2013; Shea & Pritchard, 2007). Indeed, selfesteem
is purported to be a powerful maintaining mechanism of eating
disorder psychopathology, where it has been shown to exacerbate
shape and weight concerns and dietary restraint (Fairburn et al., 2003).
It is also well-known that self-esteem is impaired in individuals with
elevated levels of social anxiety (Clark & Wells, 1995). A persistent
negative view of the self, according to the cognitive model of SAD
(Clark & Wells, 1995), is a major reason why socially anxious individuals
interpret social situations as threatening. Consistent with this,
Obeid, Bucholz, Boerner, Henderson, and Norris (2013) found that in a
sample of 344 females with an eating disorder, social anxiety had a
negative relationship with perceived global self-worth. Therefore, it is
likely that global negative self-evaluation is a primary force maintaining
symptoms of disordered eating in individuals who are socially
anxious.
Indeed, Obeid et al. (2013) and many other researchers (e.g.
Grabhorn et al., 2006; Kaye et al., 2004) utilised clinical samples in
investigating the relationship between SAD and disordered eating.
Therefore, research utilising a non-clinical population is warranted,
given the functional impact of subclinical disordered eating on individuals
(Lewinsohn, Striegel-Moore, & Seeley, 2000). Therefore, this
study aimed to explore whether reactivity to stress in social settings
and self-esteem mediate the link between social anxiety and disordered
eating in a community sample. It is hypothesised that higher levels of
social anxiety would predict higher levels of disordered eating through
a higher reactivity to stress and low self-esteem. It is hypothesised that
reactivity to stress during both social evaluations and social conflict
would mediate the relationship between social anxiety and disordered
eating.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Data was analysed from 282 online community participants. There
were 226 females (Mage=22.26, SD = 3.71) and 56 males
(Mage= 24.57, SD= 4.53). Ages ranged between 18 and 35 years.
This age range was selected given that research has suggested that this
period is when the onset of eating disorder psychopathology is most
common (Hudson, Hiripi, Pope, & Kessler, 2008). The majority of
participants (86%) reported that they lived in Australia.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Social anxiety
Social anxiety was assessed through the Liebowitz Social Anxiety
Scale (LSAS; Liebowitz, 1987).There are two subscales (social anxiety,
social avoidance), with each comprising of 24 items that are ranked on
a four-point scale, ranging from zero to three. All items are summed to
produce a total social anxiety score. Higher scores indicate higher levels
of social anxiety, with a score below 30 indicating that SAD is unlikely.
For those who met diagnosis of SAD, mean scores were found to range
between 63 and 78 (Heimberg et al., 1999). The reliability and validity
of the LSAS has been established (Heimberg et al., 1999). The LSAS
demonstrated excellent reliability for the current study (Cronbach’s
α= 0.95).
2.2.2. Disordered eating
The 26-item Eating Attitudes Test was used to assess disordered
eating (EAT-26; Garner & Garfinkel, 1979). Items are ranked on a sixpoint
scale, ranging from zero (never, rarely, sometimes) to three
(always). A total EAT-26 score is computed by summing all items and
higher scores reflect greater disordered eating severity. A score of 20 or
over is considered to be a high score, indicative of significant concerns
regarding eating and body weight and shape. The reliability and
validity of the EAT-26 has been documented (Garner, Olmsted,
Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982) and the EAT-26 demonstrated good reliability
in the current study (Cronbach’s α =0.84).
2.2.3. Stress reactivity
The social evaluations and social conflict subscales of the Perceived
Stress Reactivity Scale (PSRS; Scholtz et al., 2011) were used in the
current study. Both subscales are comprised of five items, each ranked
on a three-point scale. Items are summed to produce a subscale score,
with higher scores indicating higher levels of perceived stress reactivity.
Mean scores in a community sample aged between 26 and 60, have
been found to range between 3 and 4 for the social evaluations
subscale, and 5–6 for the social conflict subscale (Scholtz et al.,
2011). Acceptable reliability and validity has been demonstrated
(Scholtz et al., 2011). In the current sample, the social evaluations
and social conflict subscales showed the following levels of reliability,
respectively: Cronbach’s α= 0.63 and Cronbach’s α= 0.70.
J.L. Ciarma, J.M. Mathew Eating Behaviors 26 (2017) 177–181
178
2.2.4. Self-esteem
The 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem (RSE) Scale was used to assess
self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965). Each item is ranked on a four-point
scale, ranging from zero (strongly disagree) to three (strongly agree).
Items are summed to produce a total score with higher scores reflecting
higher levels of self-esteem. Scores between 15 and 25 are considered to
be within the normal range, with scores below 15 suggesting low selfesteem.
The validity and reliability of the scale has been demonstrated
(Martín-Albo, Núñez, Navarro, & Grijalvo, 2007). The RSE Scale demonstrated
good reliability for the current study (Cronbach’s α= 0.80)
2.3. Procedure
Ethics approval was granted by the Human Research Ethics
Committee and by Australian Catholic University (ACU). Participants
were recruited online via social networking websites (e.g. Facebook)
and through the National School of Psychology Research Participation
System (SONA). After being presented with the Plain Language
Statement (PLS), consent was assumed at commencement of the
questionnaire packet, which included basic demographic questions
and the study measures. Those recruited via SONA were given course
credit incentive to participate in the study, and all other participants
who completed the survey were entered into a draw to win a mini-iPad.
2.4. Data analysis
Means, standard deviations and Pearson correlation coefficients
were calculated for all variables. Mediation analyses were then
conducted to explore whether stress reactivity and self-esteem mediated
the relationship between social anxiety and disordered eating. The
significance of these indirect effects was tested via bootstrapping
procedures. Specifically, the SPSS PROCESS macro was specified to
create a total of 5000 bootstrap samples from the dataset by random
sampling with replacement (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). This generates
the indirect effects and bias-corrected confidence intervals (CI’s). When
the 95% CI’s do not include zero, then the indirect effect is significant.
Partial and full mediation was specified for significant indirect effects.
Full mediation occurs when the relationship between social anxiety and
disordered eating (i.e. the direct effect) is non-significant, while partial
mediation occurs when the relationship between social anxiety and
disordered eating is significant (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Kappa
squared is reported as the measure of effect size for indirect effects,
where values of around 0.01, 0.09, and 0.25 are considered small,
medium, and large effects, respectively (Preacher & Kelley, 2011).
3. Results
Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and correlation
coefficients for each study variable. As can be seen, social anxiety and
disordered eating is significantly and positively related, and both social
anxiety and disordered eating are positively related to the stress
reactivity subscales and negatively related to self-esteem.
Table 2 presents the indirect effect, direct effect, and effect size from
the results of the mediation analyses. As can be seen, self-esteem
(β = −0.12) and reactivity to stress during social conflict (β = 0.14)
partially mediated the relationship between social anxiety and disordered
eating. By contrast, reactivity to stress during social evaluations
(β = 0.08) did not mediate the relationship between social
anxiety and disordered eating.
4. Discussion
Although research has reported consistent links between social
anxiety and disordered eating, less is known about the mechanisms
underlying this relationship. Therefore, the purpose of the current study
was to examine whether stress reactivity and self-esteem mediates the
relationship between social anxiety and disordered eating in a community
samples.
Results from the current study demonstrate that self-esteem and a
reactivity to stress during social conflict – but not during negative social
evaluations – partially mediated the relationship between social anxiety
and disordered eating. These findings suggest that two possible reasons
why socially anxious individuals exhibit elevated symptoms of disordered
eating is through a pervasive negative view of the self and
through a higher response to stress during interpersonal conflict. These
findings are consistent with theoretical perspectives on eating disorders,
including Emotion Regulation theory, Interpersonal theory, and
the Transdiagnostic theory of eating disorders (Ansell et al., 2012;
Fairburn et al., 2003; Hilt et al., 2011; Polivy & Herman, 1993). That is,
the findings support the notion that the link between social anxiety and
disordered eating can be partially explained by difficulty coping with
stress resulting from relationship conflict. Moreover, the findings
demonstrate how low self-esteem and interpersonal conflict are powerful
mechanisms that serve to maintain eating disorder psychopathology,
and highlight the importance of ensuring that these mechanisms
are sufficiently addressed during prevention and intervention programs
(Fairburn, 2008). For example, the findings suggest that evidence based
treatment for eating disorders, such as Cognitive Behaviour Therapy –
Enhanced (CBT-E; Fairburn, 2008) may be more effective by drawing
on strategies from other treatment modalities that have a greater focus
on self-esteem, emotion regulation and interpersonal relationship
strategies.
However, unexpectedly, reactivity to stress during negative social
evaluations did not mediate the relationship between social anxiety and
disordered eating in this study. It is possible that this finding was due to
methodological limitations, where the measure used to assess stress
reactivity to social evaluations, exhibited low reliability. Further, it is
possible that differing findings between reactivity to stress during
negative social evaluations vs. relationship conflict were due to other
underlying mechanisms. For example, perhaps reactivity to stress
during relationship conflict has a stronger influence on the relationship
between social anxiety and disordered eating because some eating
Table 1
Means, standard deviation, and correlation between study variables.
Variable 1 2 3 4 5
1. Social anxiety
2. Disordered eating 0.36⁎⁎⁎
3. Stress reactivity (social
conflict)
0.28⁎⁎⁎ 0.23⁎⁎⁎
4. Stress reactivity (social
evaluations)
0.51⁎⁎⁎ 0.24⁎⁎⁎ 0.51⁎⁎⁎
5. Self-esteem −0.39⁎⁎⁎ −0.24⁎⁎⁎ −0.25⁎⁎⁎ −0.34⁎⁎⁎
Mean 51.27 8.33 6.13 5.61 16.69
Standard deviation 21.16 8.67 2.06 2.32 2.01
⁎⁎⁎ p < 0.001.
Table 2
Results from the mediation analyses.
Pathway Indirect effect
(95% CI)
Direct effect
(95% CI)
ES Type
Social anxiety → SR (social
conflict) → disordered eating
0.04 (0.01,
0.08)
0.11 (0.07,
0.16)
0.04 Partial
Social anxiety → SR (social
evaluations) →disordered
eating
0.04 (−0.02,
0.10)
0.11 (0.06,
0.16)
0.03 None
Social anxiety → self-esteem →
disordered eating
0.05 (0.01,
0.09)
0.11 (0.07,
0.15)
0.05 Partial
Note. Indirect effect refers to standardised values; ES= effect size (Kappa squared);
SR =stress reactivity.
J.L. Ciarma, J.M. Mathew Eating Behaviors 26 (2017) 177–181
179
disorders (i.e. Anorexia Nervosa) are characterised by low body weight
that can be recognised by others, and in turn, elicit concern and care. In
a way, eating disorder symptoms could then also serve the purpose of
non-verbal means to attempt to resolve relationship conflict. However,
this may not be the case for reactivity to negative social evaluations.
Another important finding was that partial rather than full mediation
was observed for self-esteem and reactivity to stress during social
conflict. This demonstrates the importance of discovering the additional
mechanisms that account for the relationship between social anxiety
and disordered eating. Therefore, future research would benefit from
testing a more complex model that takes into account other potential
moderating and mediating variables of the relationship between social
anxiety and disordered eating.
There are limitations to the current study that need to be considered.
The cross-sectional and correlational nature of this study precludes
inferences made about the direction of the relationships
specified and also about causality. Although cross-sectional designs
are an important first-step to understand plausible mechanisms underlying
the relationship between social anxiety and disordered eating,
future longitudinal research is clearly needed to test direction of the
relationships specified in the models present in this study. In addition,
our sample was predominately female (80%) and therefore findings
from the current study may not be generalised to the male population.
It is important for future research to examine the impact of stress
reactivity and self-esteem on social anxiety and disordered eating in
large male samples, as social anxiety and disordered eating is becoming
increasingly prevalent in males (Strother, Lemberg,
Stanford, & Turberville, 2012; Xu et al., 2012). Further, the study was
limited in that participants were recruited online via social media
websites, which may have introduced bias in the sample (e.g. by
attracting those of higher socio-economic status with access to a
computer and internet). Lastly, given that participants could complete
questionnaires in their own time and at a chosen location, the study did
not control other extraneous variables such as distractions, fatigue, and
time pressure.
In sum, low self-esteem and a higher reactivity to stress during
social conflict partially mediated the relationship between social
anxiety and disordered eating in a community sample. These findings
suggest that increasing focus on strategies related to self-esteem and
interpersonal relationships in current treatment approaches to eating
disorders, may enhance their effectiveness. Further longitudinal research
is needed to examine the directionality of the proposed models.
References
Ansell, E. B., Grilo, C. M., & White, M. (2012). Examining the interpersonal model of
binge eating and loss of control over eating in women. The International Journal of
Eating Disorders, 45(1), 43–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20897.
Barker, E. T., & Galambos, N. L. (2007). Body dissatisfaction, living away from parents,
and poor social adjustment predict binge eating symptoms in young women making
the transition to university. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(7), 904–911. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-006-9134-6.
Brewerton, T. D., Lydiard, R. B., Herzog, D. B., Brotman, A. W., O’Neil, P. M., & Ballenger,
J. C. (1995). Comorbidity of Axis I psychiatric disorders in Bulimia Nervosa. Journal
of Clinical Psychiatry, 56(2), 77–80 (Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/
pubmed/7852257).
Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P. F., Fear, J., & Joyce, P. R. (1997). Eating disorders and
antecedent anxiety disorders: A controlled study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
96(2), 101–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb09913.x.
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg,
M. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment
and treatment (pp. 69–93). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Delinsky, S. S., & Wilson, G. T. (2008). Weight gain, dietary restraint, and disordered
eating in the freshman year of college. Eating Behaviors, 9(1), 82–90. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.06.001.
Fairburn, C. G. (2008). Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating
disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
41, 509–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00088-8.
Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1979). The Eating attitudes Test: An index of the
symptoms of Anorexia Nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 9(2), 273–279. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1017/s0033291700030762.
Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The Eating attitudes
Test: Psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine, 12(4),
871–878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/2FS0033291700049163.
Goodwin, R. D., & Fitzgibbon, M. L. (2002). Social anxiety as a barrier to treatment for
eating disorders. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32(1), 103–106.
Retrieved from http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.
cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=med4&NEWS=N&AN=12183936.
Grabhorn, M., Stenner, H., Stangier, U., & Kaufhold, A. (2006). Social anxiety in anorexia
and bulimia nervosa: The mediating role of shame. Clinical
Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13(1), 12–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp.463.
Halmi, K. A., Eckert, E., Marchi, P., Sampugnaro, V., Apple, R., & Cohen, J. (1991).
Comorbidity of psychiatric diagnoses in Anorexia Nervosa. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 48(8), 712–718. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1991.
01810320036006.
Hatch, A., Madden, S., Kohn, M., Clarke, S., Touyz, S., & Williams, L. M. (2010). Anorexia
nervosa: Towards an integrative neuroscience model. The Journal of the Eating
Disorders Association, 18(3), 165–179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.974.
Heimberg, R. G., Horner, K. J., Juster, H. R., Safren, S. A., Brown, E. J., Schneier, F. R., &
Liebowitz, M. R. (1999). Psychometric properties of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety
Scale. Psychological Medicine, 29(1), 199–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
S0033291798007879.
Hilt, L. M., Hanson, J. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2011). Emotion dysregulation. Encyclopedia of
Adolescence, 3(1), 160–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-373915-5.
00112-1.
Hinrichsen, H., Wright, F., Waller, G., & Meyer, C. (2003). Social anxiety and coping
strategies in the eating disorders. Eating Behaviors, 4(2), 117–126. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00016-3.
Hudson, J. I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H. G., & Kessler, R. C. (2008). The prevalence and
correlates of eating disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication.
Biological Psychiatry, 61(3), 348–358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.
040.
Kaye, W. H., Bulik, C. M., Thornton, L., Barbarich, N., & Masters, K. (2004). Comorbidity
of anxiety disorders with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 161(12), 2215–2221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.161.12.2215.
Klump, K., Perkins, P. S., Burt, S. A., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Puberty
moderates genetic influences on disordered eating. Psychological Medicine, 37(5),
627–634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291707000189.
Lampard, M. A., Byrne, S., McLean, N., & Fursland, A. (2011). An evaluation of the
enhanced cognitive-behavioural model of Bulimia Nervosa. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 49(9), 529–535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.002.
Lampard, M. A., Tasca, G., Balfour, L., & Bissada, H. (2013). An evaluation of the
transdiagnostic cognitive-behavioural model of eating disorders. European Eating
Disorders Review, 21(2), 99–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.2214.
Lask, B. (2000). Aetiology. In B. Lask, & R. Bryanyt-Waugh (Eds.), Anorexia nervosa and
eating disorders in childhood and adolescence (pp. 63–79). East Sussex: Psychology
Press.
Levinson, C. A., & Rodebaugh, T. L. (2012). Social anxiety and eating disorder
comorbidity: The role of negative social evaluation fears. Eating Behaviors, 13(1),
27–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.11.006.
Lewinsohn, P. M., Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Seeley, J. R. (2000). Epidemiology and natural
course of eating disorders in young women from adolescence to young adulthood.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1284–1292.
Retrieved from www.ovid.com.
Lieberman, M., Gauvin, L., Bukowski, V. M., & White, D. R. (2001). Interpersonal
influence and disordered eating behaviors in adolescent girls: The role of peer
modeling, social reinforcement, and body-related teasing. Eating Behaviours, 2(3),
215–236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1471-0153(01)00030-7.
Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). Social phobia. Modern Problems of Pharmacopsychiatry, 22,
141–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/t07671-000.
Martín-Albo, J., Núñez, J. L., Navarro, J. G., & Grijalvo, F. (2007). The Rosenberg Self-
Esteem Scale: Translation and validation in university students. The Spanish Journal of
Psychology, 10(2), 458–467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1138741600006727.
McLean, C. P., Miller, N. A., & Hope, D. A. (2007). Mediating social anxiety and
disordered eating: The role of expressive suppression. Eating Disorders, 15(1), 41–54.
Retrieved from http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.
cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=med5&NEWS=N&AN=17162640.
Menatti, A. R., DeBoer, L. B. H., Weeks, J. W., & Heimberg, R. G. (2015). Social anxiety
and associations with eating psychopathology: Mediating effects of fears of
evaluation. Body Image, 14, 20–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.02.
003.
Obeid, N., Bucholz, A., Boerner, K. E., Henderson, K. A., & Norris, M. (2013). Self-esteem
and social anxiety in an adolescent female eating disorder population: Age and
diagnostic effects. Eating Disorders, 21(2), 140–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
10640266.2013.761088.
Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1993). Etiology of binge eating: Psychological mechanisms. In
C. G. Fairburn, & G. T. Wilson (Eds.), Binge eating: Nature, assessment, and treatment
(pp. 173–205). New York: Guilford Press.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing
and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research
Methods, 40(3), 879–891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/brm.40.3.879.
Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models:
Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods,
16(2), 93–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022658.
Robson, P. J. (1988). Self-esteem: A psychiatric view. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153(1),
6–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.153.1.6.
J.L. Ciarma, J.M. Mathew Eating Behaviors 26 (2017) 177–181
180
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Ruscio, A. M., Brown, T. A., Chiu, W. T., Sareen, J., Stein, M. B., & Kessler, R. C. (2008).
Social fears and social phobia in the USA: Results from the national comorbidity survey
replication. 15–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707001699.
Scholtz, W., Yim, I. S., Zoccola, P. M., Jansen, L., & Schulz, P. (2011). The perceived stress
reactivity scale: Measurement invariance, stability, and validity in three countries.
Psychological Assessment, 23, 80–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021148.
Shea, M. E., & Pritchard, M. E. (2007). Is self-esteem the primary predictor of disordered
eating? Personality and Individual Differences, 42(8), 1527–1537. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.026.
Strother, E., Lemberg, R., Stanford, S. C., & Turberville, D. (2012). Eating disorders in
men: Underdiagnosed, undertreated, and misunderstood. Eating Disorders, 20(5),
346–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10640266.2012.715512.
Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Vögele, C. (1999). Psychological and physiological reactivity to
stress: An experimental study on bulimic patients, restrained eaters and controls.
Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 68(6), 333–340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/
000012352.
Wonderlich-Tierney, A. L., & Vander Wal, J. S. (2010). The effects of social support and
coping on the relationship between social anxiety and eating disorders. Eating
Behaviors, 11(2), 85–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.10.002.
Xu, Y., Schneier, F., Heimberg, R. G., Princisvalle, K., Liebowitz, M. R., Wang, S., &
Blanco, C. (2012). Gender differences in social anxiety disorder: Results from the
national epidemiologic sample on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 26(1), 12–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.006.
J.L. Ciarma, J.M. Mathew Eating Behaviors 26 (2017) 177–181
181


Get Professional Assignment Help Cheaply

Buy Custom Essay

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Social anxiety and disordered eating
Just from $9/Page
Order Essay

Are you busy and do not have time to handle your assignment? Are you scared that your paper will not make the grade? Do you have responsibilities that may hinder you from turning in your assignment on time? Are you tired and can barely handle your assignment? Are your grades inconsistent?

Whichever your reason is, it is valid! You can get professional academic help from our service at affordable rates. We have a team of professional academic writers who can handle all your assignments.

Why Choose Our Academic Writing Service?

  • Plagiarism free papers
  • Timely delivery
  • Any deadline
  • Skilled, Experienced Native English Writers
  • Subject-relevant academic writer
  • Adherence to paper instructions
  • Ability to tackle bulk assignments
  • Reasonable prices
  • 24/7 Customer Support
  • Get superb grades consistently

Online Academic Help With Different Subjects

Literature

Students barely have time to read. We got you! Have your literature essay or book review written without having the hassle of reading the book. You can get your literature paper custom-written for you by our literature specialists.

Finance

Do you struggle with finance? No need to torture yourself if finance is not your cup of tea. You can order your finance paper from our academic writing service and get 100% original work from competent finance experts.

Computer science

Computer science is a tough subject. Fortunately, our computer science experts are up to the match. No need to stress and have sleepless nights. Our academic writers will tackle all your computer science assignments and deliver them on time. Let us handle all your python, java, ruby, JavaScript, php , C+ assignments!

Psychology

While psychology may be an interesting subject, you may lack sufficient time to handle your assignments. Don’t despair; by using our academic writing service, you can be assured of perfect grades. Moreover, your grades will be consistent.

Engineering

Engineering is quite a demanding subject. Students face a lot of pressure and barely have enough time to do what they love to do. Our academic writing service got you covered! Our engineering specialists follow the paper instructions and ensure timely delivery of the paper.

Nursing

In the nursing course, you may have difficulties with literature reviews, annotated bibliographies, critical essays, and other assignments. Our nursing assignment writers will offer you professional nursing paper help at low prices.

Sociology

Truth be told, sociology papers can be quite exhausting. Our academic writing service relieves you of fatigue, pressure, and stress. You can relax and have peace of mind as our academic writers handle your sociology assignment.

Business

We take pride in having some of the best business writers in the industry. Our business writers have a lot of experience in the field. They are reliable, and you can be assured of a high-grade paper. They are able to handle business papers of any subject, length, deadline, and difficulty!

Statistics

We boast of having some of the most experienced statistics experts in the industry. Our statistics experts have diverse skills, expertise, and knowledge to handle any kind of assignment. They have access to all kinds of software to get your assignment done.

Law

Writing a law essay may prove to be an insurmountable obstacle, especially when you need to know the peculiarities of the legislative framework. Take advantage of our top-notch law specialists and get superb grades and 100% satisfaction.

What discipline/subjects do you deal in?

We have highlighted some of the most popular subjects we handle above. Those are just a tip of the iceberg. We deal in all academic disciplines since our writers are as diverse. They have been drawn from across all disciplines, and orders are assigned to those writers believed to be the best in the field. In a nutshell, there is no task we cannot handle; all you need to do is place your order with us. As long as your instructions are clear, just trust we shall deliver irrespective of the discipline.

Are your writers competent enough to handle my paper?

Our essay writers are graduates with bachelor's, masters, Ph.D., and doctorate degrees in various subjects. The minimum requirement to be an essay writer with our essay writing service is to have a college degree. All our academic writers have a minimum of two years of academic writing. We have a stringent recruitment process to ensure that we get only the most competent essay writers in the industry. We also ensure that the writers are handsomely compensated for their value. The majority of our writers are native English speakers. As such, the fluency of language and grammar is impeccable.

What if I don’t like the paper?

There is a very low likelihood that you won’t like the paper.

Reasons being:

  • When assigning your order, we match the paper’s discipline with the writer’s field/specialization. Since all our writers are graduates, we match the paper’s subject with the field the writer studied. For instance, if it’s a nursing paper, only a nursing graduate and writer will handle it. Furthermore, all our writers have academic writing experience and top-notch research skills.
  • We have a quality assurance that reviews the paper before it gets to you. As such, we ensure that you get a paper that meets the required standard and will most definitely make the grade.

In the event that you don’t like your paper:

  • The writer will revise the paper up to your pleasing. You have unlimited revisions. You simply need to highlight what specifically you don’t like about the paper, and the writer will make the amendments. The paper will be revised until you are satisfied. Revisions are free of charge
  • We will have a different writer write the paper from scratch.
  • Last resort, if the above does not work, we will refund your money.

Will the professor find out I didn’t write the paper myself?

Not at all. All papers are written from scratch. There is no way your tutor or instructor will realize that you did not write the paper yourself. In fact, we recommend using our assignment help services for consistent results.

What if the paper is plagiarized?

We check all papers for plagiarism before we submit them. We use powerful plagiarism checking software such as SafeAssign, LopesWrite, and Turnitin. We also upload the plagiarism report so that you can review it. We understand that plagiarism is academic suicide. We would not take the risk of submitting plagiarized work and jeopardize your academic journey. Furthermore, we do not sell or use prewritten papers, and each paper is written from scratch.

When will I get my paper?

You determine when you get the paper by setting the deadline when placing the order. All papers are delivered within the deadline. We are well aware that we operate in a time-sensitive industry. As such, we have laid out strategies to ensure that the client receives the paper on time and they never miss the deadline. We understand that papers that are submitted late have some points deducted. We do not want you to miss any points due to late submission. We work on beating deadlines by huge margins in order to ensure that you have ample time to review the paper before you submit it.

Will anyone find out that I used your services?

We have a privacy and confidentiality policy that guides our work. We NEVER share any customer information with third parties. Noone will ever know that you used our assignment help services. It’s only between you and us. We are bound by our policies to protect the customer’s identity and information. All your information, such as your names, phone number, email, order information, and so on, are protected. We have robust security systems that ensure that your data is protected. Hacking our systems is close to impossible, and it has never happened.

How our Assignment Help Service Works

1. Place an order

You fill all the paper instructions in the order form. Make sure you include all the helpful materials so that our academic writers can deliver the perfect paper. It will also help to eliminate unnecessary revisions.

2. Pay for the order

Proceed to pay for the paper so that it can be assigned to one of our expert academic writers. The paper subject is matched with the writer’s area of specialization.

3. Track the progress

You communicate with the writer and know about the progress of the paper. The client can ask the writer for drafts of the paper. The client can upload extra material and include additional instructions from the lecturer. Receive a paper.

4. Download the paper

The paper is sent to your email and uploaded to your personal account. You also get a plagiarism report attached to your paper.

smile and order essay GET A PERFECT SCORE!!! smile and order essay Buy Custom Essay


Place your order
(550 words)

Approximate price: $22

Calculate the price of your order

550 words
We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
Total price:
$26
The price is based on these factors:
Academic level
Number of pages
Urgency
Basic features
  • Free title page and bibliography
  • Unlimited revisions
  • Plagiarism-free guarantee
  • Money-back guarantee
  • 24/7 support
On-demand options
  • Writer’s samples
  • Part-by-part delivery
  • Overnight delivery
  • Copies of used sources
  • Expert Proofreading
Paper format
  • 275 words per page
  • 12 pt Arial/Times New Roman
  • Double line spacing
  • Any citation style (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard)

Our guarantees

Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.

Money-back guarantee

You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.

Read more

Zero-plagiarism guarantee

Each paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.

Read more

Free-revision policy

Thanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.

Read more

Privacy policy

Your email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.

Read more

Fair-cooperation guarantee

By sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.

Read more
error: Content is protected !!
Open chat
1
Need assignment help? You can contact our live agent via WhatsApp using +1 718 717 2861

Feel free to ask questions, clarifications, or discounts available when placing an order.
  +1 718 717 2861           + 44 161 818 7126           [email protected]
  +1 718 717 2861         [email protected]